I Dreamt of Meangaling With People I Have Been Ask Not to See Again What Does That Mean

Consequence occurring in the mind while sleeping

A Dream of a Daughter Before a Sunrise c. 1830–33 by Karl Bryullov (1799–1852)

A dream is a succession of images, ideas, emotions, and sensations that commonly occur involuntarily in the mind during certain stages of sleep.[1] Humans spend nigh two hours dreaming per dark,[2] and each dream lasts around 5 to 20 minutes, although the dreamer may perceive the dream every bit beingness much longer than this.[3]

The content and function of dreams have been topics of scientific, philosophical and religious interest throughout recorded history. Dream interpretation, practiced by the Babylonians in the tertiary millennium BCE[4] and even earlier by the ancient Sumerians,[v] [6] figures prominently in religious texts in several traditions, and has played a lead role in psychotherapy.[seven] [8] The scientific study of dreams is called oneirology.[9] Most modern dream study focuses on the neurophysiology of dreams and on proposing and testing hypotheses regarding dream function. Information technology is not known where in the brain dreams originate, if at that place is a unmarried origin for dreams or if multiple regions of the brain are involved, or what the purpose of dreaming is for the body or mind.

The man dream feel and what to make of it has undergone sizable shifts over the course of history.[10] [xi] Long ago, co-ordinate to writings from Mesopotamia and Ancient Egypt, dreams dictated post-dream behaviors to an extent sharply reduced in later millennia. These ancient writings nearly dreams highlight visitation dreams, where a dream figure, usually a deity or a prominent forebear, commands the dreamer to take specific deportment and may predict future events.[12] [13] [xiv] The brain action capable of formulating such dreams, rare amongst literate people in later eras, conforms to the bicameral mentality hypothesized past Julian Jaynes as ascendant into the second or kickoff millennium BCE. Framing the dream experience varies across cultures as well as through time.

Dreaming and sleep are intertwined. Dreams occur mainly in the rapid-eye movement (REM) stage of slumber—when brain activity is high and resembles that of being awake. Because REM sleep is detectable in many species, and because research suggests that all mammals feel REM,[15] linking dreams to REM sleep has led to conjectures that animals dream. Even so, humans dream during non-REM sleep, also, and not all REM awakenings elicit dream reports.[sixteen] To be studied, a dream must first be reduced to a verbal report, which is an account of the bailiwick'due south retentiveness of the dream, not the field of study's dream experience itself. And so, dreaming by non-humans is currently unprovable, equally is dreaming by man fetuses and pre-verbal infants.[17]

Subjective experience

Preserved writings from early Mediterranean civilizations signal a relatively abrupt alter in subjective dream experience between Bronze Age artifact and the beginnings of the classical era.[xviii]

In visitation dreams reported in aboriginal writings, dreamers were largely passive in their dreams, and visual content served primarily to frame authoritative auditory messaging.[xix] [x] [20] Gudea, the king of the Sumerian city-land of Lagash (reigned c. 2144–2124 BCE), rebuilt the temple of Ningirsu every bit the result of a dream in which he was told to do so.[vi] After antiquity, the passive hearing of visitation dreams largely gave way to visualized narratives in which the dreamer becomes a character who actively participates.

From the 1940s to 1985, Calvin S. Hall nerveless more fifty,000 dream reports at Western Reserve University. In 1966, Hall and Robert Van de Castle published The Content Assay of Dreams, in which they outlined a coding system to study 1,000 dream reports from college students.[21] Results indicated that participants from varying parts of the earth demonstrated similarity in their dream content. The only residue of antiquity's authoritative dream figure in the Hall and Van de Castle list of dream characters is the inclusion of God in the category of prominent persons.[22] Hall's complete dream reports were made publicly available in the mid-1990s by his protégé William Domhoff. More contempo studies of dream reports, while providing more detail, continue to cite the Hall written report favorably.[23]

A soldier dreams: the trenches of WWI. Jan Styka (1858–1925).

In the Hall report, the most mutual emotion experienced in dreams was anxiety. Other emotions included abandonment, acrimony, fear, joy, and happiness. Negative emotions were much more mutual than positive ones.[21] The Hall data analysis showed that sexual dreams occur no more than 10% of the time and are more than prevalent in young to mid-teens.[21] Another study showed that viii% of both men's and women'south dreams accept sexual content.[24] In some cases, sexual dreams may result in orgasms or nocturnal emissions. These are colloquially known as "wet dreams."[25]

The visual nature of dreams is mostly highly phantasmagoric; that is, different locations and objects continuously blend into each other. The visuals (including locations, people, and objects) are mostly reflective of a person'due south memories and experiences, only conversation tin take on highly exaggerated and baroque forms. Some dreams may even tell elaborate stories wherein the dreamer enters entirely new, complex worlds and awakes with ideas, thoughts and feelings never experienced prior to the dream.

People who are blind from birth exercise not accept visual dreams. Their dream contents are related to other senses like hearing, affect, smell and taste, whichever are present since birth.[26]

Neurophysiology

Dream study is popular with scientists exploring the mind–encephalon problem. Some "propose to reduce aspects of dream phenomenology to neurobiology."[27] But current science cannot specify dream physiology in detail. Protocols in most nations restrict human being brain research to non-invasive procedures. In the United States, invasive brain procedures with a human subject field are allowed just when these are accounted necessary in surgical treatment to address medical needs of the same human being field of study.[28] Not-invasive measures of encephalon action like electroencephalogram (EEG) voltage averaging or cognitive blood flow cannot identify small but influential neuronal populations.[29] Too, fMRI signals are likewise slow to explain how brains compute in real time.[30]

Scientists researching some brain functions can piece of work effectually current restrictions by examining animal subjects. As stated by the Social club for Neuroscience, "Because no adequate alternatives exist, much of this research must be done on animate being subjects."[31] Yet, since animal dreaming can be only inferred, not confirmed, animal studies yield no hard facts to illuminate the neurophysiology of dreams. Examining homo subjects with encephalon lesions can provide clues, only the lesion method cannot discriminate between the effects of destruction and disconnection and cannot target specific neuronal groups in heterogeneous regions like the brain stem.[29]

Generation

Denied precision tools, obliged to depend on imaging, much dream enquiry has succumbed to the law of the instrument. Studies detect an increase of blood flow in a specific encephalon region and and so credit that region with a role in generating dreams. But pooling study results has led to the newer decision that dreaming involves large numbers of regions and pathways, which likely are different for different dream events.[32]

Image creation in the brain involves meaning neural activeness downstream from eye intake, and it is theorized that "the visual imagery of dreams is produced by activation during sleep of the aforementioned structures that generate complex visual imagery in waking perception."[33]

Dreams present a running narrative rather than exclusively visual imagery. Post-obit their piece of work with divide-brain subjects, Gazzaniga and LeDoux postulated, without attempting to specify the neural mechanisms, a "left-encephalon interpreter" that seeks to create a plausible narrative from whatever electro-chemic signals reach the brain'south left hemisphere. Sleep research has adamant that some brain regions fully active during waking are, during REM slumber, activated only in a partial or fragmentary way.[34] Drawing on this knowledge, textbook author James Westward. Kalat explains, "[A] dream represents the encephalon's effort to make sense of sparse and distorted information.... The cortex combines this haphazard input with whatever other action was already occurring and does its best to synthesize a story that makes sense of the data."[35] Neuroscientist Indre Viskontas is fifty-fifty more blunt, calling often bizarre dream content "but the issue of your interpreter trying to create a story out of random neural signaling."[36]

Theories on function

For humans in the pre-classical era, and continuing for some not-literate populations into mod times, dreams are believed to accept functioned as revealers of truths sourced during sleep from gods or other external entities.[37] [xi] Aboriginal Egyptians believed that dreams were the all-time way to receive divine revelation, and thus they would induce (or "incubate") dreams. They went to sanctuaries and slept on special "dream beds" in hope of receiving advice, comfort, or healing from the gods.[xiv] From a Darwinian perspective dreams would have to fulfill some kind of biological requirement, provide some benefit for natural choice to take place, or at least have no negative touch on on fitness. Robert (1886),[38] a physician from Hamburg, was the first who suggested that dreams are a need and that they accept the function to erase (a) sensory impressions that were not fully worked upwards, and (b) ideas that were non fully developed during the mean solar day. In dreams, incomplete material is either removed (suppressed) or deepened and included into memory. Freud, whose dream studies focused on interpreting dreams, not explaining how or why humans dream, disputed Robert's hypothesis[39] and proposed that dreams preserve slumber by representing as fulfilled those wishes that otherwise would awaken the dreamer.[xl] Freud wrote that dreams "serve the purpose of prolonging sleep instead of waking upwardly. Dreams are the GUARDIANS of sleep and non its disturbers."[41]

A turning point in theorizing about dream role came in 1953, when Science published the Aserinsky and Kleitman paper[42] establishing REM sleep as a distinct phase of sleep and linking dreams to REM slumber.[43] Until and even after publication of the Solms 2000 paper that certified the separability of REM sleep and dream phenomena,[xvi] many studies purporting to uncover the function of dreams accept in fact been studying not dreams but measurable REM slumber.

Theories of dream role since the identification of REM slumber include:

Hobson's and McCarley'due south 1977 activation-synthesis hypothesis, which proposed "a functional part for dreaming sleep in promoting some aspect of the learning process...."[44] In 2010 a Harvard study was published showing experimental show that dreams were correlated with improved learning.[45]

Crick's and Mitchison's 1983 "reverse learning" theory, which states that dreams are like the cleaning-upwards operations of computers when they are offline, removing (suppressing) parasitic nodes and other "junk" from the listen during slumber.[46] [47]

Hartmann'southward 1995 proposal that dreams serve a "quasi-therapeutic" function, enabling the dreamer to procedure trauma in a safety place.[48]

Revonsuo's 2000 threat simulation hypothesis, whose premise is that during much of human being evolution, physical and interpersonal threats were serious, giving reproductive reward to those who survived them. Dreaming aided survival by replicating these threats and providing the dreamer with practice in dealing with them.[49]

Eagleman's and Vaughn'southward 2021 defensive activation theory, which says that, given the encephalon's neuroplasticity, dreams evolved as a visual hallucinatory activeness during sleep's extended periods of darkness, busying the occipital lobe and thereby protecting it from possible cribbing past other, not-vision, sense operations.[50]

Religious and other cultural contexts

Dreams figure prominently in major world religions. The dream experience for early humans, co-ordinate to one interpretation, gave rise to the notion of a human "soul,"[51] a cardinal element in much religious thought. J. W. Dunne wrote:

But there tin be no reasonable doubt that the idea of a soul must take outset arisen in the mind of primitive man as a result of observation of his dreams. Ignorant as he was, he could have come to no other determination but that, in dreams, he left his sleeping trunk in one universe and went wandering off into another. It is considered that, only for that cruel, the thought of such a thing as a 'soul' would never have even occurred to mankind....[52]

Hindu

In the Mandukya Upanishad, part of the Veda scriptures of Indian Hinduism, a dream is i of three states that the soul experiences during its lifetime, the other two states being the waking land and the sleep state.[53] The earliest Upanishads, written earlier 300 BCE, emphasize 2 meanings of dreams. The first says that dreams are just expressions of inner desires. The second is the belief of the soul leaving the torso and being guided until awakened.

Abrahamic

In Judaism, dreams are considered role of the feel of the earth that can be interpreted and from which lessons tin can be garnered. It is discussed in the Talmud, Tractate Berachot 55–threescore.

The ancient Hebrews connected their dreams heavily with their religion, though the Hebrews were monotheistic and believed that dreams were the voice of one God alone. Hebrews also differentiated between good dreams (from God) and bad dreams (from evil spirits). The Hebrews, like many other aboriginal cultures, incubated dreams in order to receive a divine revelation. For example, the Hebrew prophet Samuel would "prevarication down and sleep in the temple at Shiloh before the Ark and receive the word of the Lord." Most of the dreams in the Bible are in the Volume of Genesis.[54]

Christians mostly shared the beliefs of the Hebrews and thought that dreams were of a supernatural character considering the One-time Attestation includes frequent stories of dreams with divine inspiration. The most famous of these dream stories was Jacob's dream of a ladder that stretches from Globe to Heaven. Many Christians preach that God can speak to people through their dreams. The famous glossary, the Somniale Danielis, written in the name of Daniel, attempted to teach Christian populations to interpret their dreams.

Iain R. Edgar has researched the office of dreams in Islam.[55] He has argued that dreams play an important role in the history of Islam and the lives of Muslims, since dream interpretation is the simply fashion that Muslims can receive revelations from God since the decease of the last prophet, Muhammad.[56] According to Edgar, Islam classifies three types of dreams. Firstly, there is the true dream (al-ru'ya), then the false dream, which may come from the devil (shaytan), and finally, the meaningless everyday dream (hulm). This last dream could be brought forth by the dreamer's ego or base appetite based on what they experienced in the real world. The true dream is oft indicated past Islam's hadith tradition.[56] In i narration by Aisha, the wife of the Prophet, it is said that the Prophet's dreams would come up true similar the ocean's waves.[56] Merely as in its predecessors, the Quran likewise recounts the story of Joseph and his unique ability to interpret dreams.[56]

Buddhist

In Buddhism, ideas well-nigh dreams are similar to the classical and folk traditions in Southern asia. The aforementioned dream is sometimes experienced by multiple people, every bit in the case of the Buddha-to-be, before he is leaving his home. Information technology is described in the Mahāvastu that several of the Buddha's relatives had premonitory dreams preceding this. Some dreams are also seen to transcend time: the Buddha-to-be has certain dreams that are the same every bit those of previous Buddhas, the Lalitavistara states. In Buddhist literature, dreams oftentimes function as a "signpost" motif to mark certain stages in the life of the chief graphic symbol.[57]

Buddhist views about dreams are expressed in the Pāli Commentaries and the Milinda Pañhā.[57]

Other

In Chinese history, people wrote of two vital aspects of the soul of which one is freed from the body during slumber to journey in a dream realm, while the other remained in the torso.[58] This belief and dream interpretation had been questioned since early times, such as by the philosopher Wang Chong (27–97CE).[58]

The Babylonians and Assyrians divided dreams into "good," which were sent past the gods, and "bad," sent by demons.[59] A surviving drove of dream omens entitled Iškar Zaqīqu records various dream scenarios besides as prognostications of what will happen to the person who experiences each dream, evidently based on previous cases.[6] [sixty] Some list different possible outcomes, based on occasions in which people experienced like dreams with different results.[6] The Greeks shared their behavior with the Egyptians on how to translate good and bad dreams, and the idea of incubating dreams. Morpheus, the Greek god of dreams, also sent warnings and prophecies to those who slept at shrines and temples. The earliest Greek beliefs about dreams were that their gods physically visited the dreamers, where they entered through a keyhole, exiting the same way later the divine message was given.

Antiphon wrote the first known Greek book on dreams in the 5th century BCE. In that century, other cultures influenced Greeks to develop the conventionalities that souls left the sleeping body.[61] Hippocrates (469–399BCE) had a simple dream theory: during the day, the soul receives images; during the dark, it produces images. Greek philosopher Aristotle (384–322 BCE) believed dreams caused physiological activeness. He thought dreams could clarify illness and predict diseases. Marcus Tullius Cicero, for his office, believed that all dreams are produced by thoughts and conversations a dreamer had during the preceding days.[62] Cicero's Somnium Scipionis described a lengthy dream vision, which in turn was commented on past Macrobius in his Commentarii in Somnium Scipionis.

Herodotus in his The Histories, writes "The visions that occur to us in dreams are, by and large, the things nosotros have been concerned about during the mean solar day."[63]

The Dreaming is a mutual term inside the animist creation narrative of indigenous Australians for a personal, or grouping, creation and for what may be understood as the "timeless time" of formative creation and perpetual creating.[64]

Some Indigenous American tribes and Mexican populations believe that dreams are a way of visiting and having contact with their ancestors.[65] Some Native American tribes have used vision quests as a rite of passage, fasting and praying until an predictable guiding dream was received, to exist shared with the remainder of the tribe upon their return.[66] [67]

Interpretation

Starting time in the late 19th century, Austrian neurologist Sigmund Freud, founder of psychoanalysis, theorized that dreams reflect the dreamer's unconscious heed and specifically that dream content is shaped past unconscious wish fulfillment. He argued that of import unconscious desires frequently relate to early babyhood memories and experiences.[7] Carl Jung and others expanded on Freud's thought that dream content reflects the dreamer's unconscious desires.

Dream estimation tin can exist a result of subjective ideas and experiences. One study constitute that most people believe that "their dreams reveal meaningful subconscious truths."[68] The researchers surveyed students in the Us, Southward Korea, and India, and found that 74% of Indians, 65% of South Koreans and 56% of Americans believed their dream content provided them with meaningful insight into their unconscious beliefs and desires. This Freudian view of dreaming was believed significantly more than theories of dreaming that attribute dream content to retentiveness consolidation, problem-solving, or as a byproduct of unrelated brain activity. The same written report found that people attribute more importance to dream content than to similar thought content that occurs while they are awake. Americans were more than likely to study that they would miss their flight if they dreamt of their plane crashing than if they thought of their plane crashing the night before flight (while awake), and that they would exist as likely to miss their flight if they dreamt of their plane crashing the nighttime before their flight as if in that location was an actual plane crash on the route they intended to have. Participants in the report were more than likely to perceive dreams to be meaningful when the content of dreams was in accordance with their beliefs and desires while awake. They were more than likely to view a positive dream about a friend to be meaningful than a positive dream most someone they disliked, for example, and were more than likely to view a negative dream about a person they disliked as meaningful than a negative dream nearly a person they liked.

According to surveys, information technology is common for people to experience their dreams are predicting subsequent life events.[69] Psychologists accept explained these experiences in terms of memory biases, namely a selective memory for accurate predictions and distorted memory so that dreams are retrospectively fitted onto life experiences.[69] The multi-faceted nature of dreams makes it easy to observe connections between dream content and real events.[70] The term "veridical dream" has been used to indicate dreams that reveal or contain truths non yet known to the dreamer, whether future events or secrets.[71]

In 1 experiment, subjects were asked to write down their dreams in a diary. This prevented the selective memory result, and the dreams no longer seemed accurate almost the time to come.[72] Another experiment gave subjects a fake diary of a student with patently precognitive dreams. This diary described events from the person's life, as well equally some predictive dreams and some non-predictive dreams. When subjects were asked to recollect the dreams they had read, they remembered more of the successful predictions than unsuccessful ones.[73]

Images and literature

Graphic artists, writers and filmmakers all have found dreams to offering a rich vein for creative expression. In the W, artists' depictions of dreams in Renaissance and Bizarre fine art often were related to Biblical narrative. Especially preferred past visual artists were the Jacob'southward Ladder dream in Genesis and St. Joseph's dreams in the Gospel co-ordinate to Matthew.

Many later graphic artists have depicted dreams, including Japanese woodblock artist Hokusai (1760–1849) and Western European painters Rousseau (1844–1910), Picasso (1881–1973), and Dali (1904–1989).

In literature, dream frames were frequently used in medieval allegory to justify the narrative; The Book of the Duchess [74] and The Vision Concerning Piers Plowman [75] are 2 such dream visions. Even before them, in antiquity, the aforementioned device had been used by Cicero and Lucian of Samosata.

The cheshire cat, John Tenniel (1820–1914), illustration in Alice'southward Adventures in Wonderland, 1866 edition.

Dreams have likewise featured in fantasy and speculative fiction since the 19th century. One of the best-known dream worlds is Wonderland from Lewis Carroll's Alice's Adventures in Wonderland, as well as Looking-Glass Country from its sequel, Through the Looking-Drinking glass. Unlike many dream worlds, Carroll's logic is like that of actual dreams, with transitions and flexible causality.

Other fictional dream worlds include the Dreamlands of H. P. Lovecraft'southward Dream Cycle [76] and The Neverending Story 's[77] world of Fantastica, which includes places like the Desert of Lost Dreams, the Sea of Possibilities and the Swamps of Sadness. Dreamworlds, shared hallucinations and other alternating realities feature in a number of works by Philip Chiliad. Dick, such as The Three Stigmata of Palmer Eldritch and Ubik. Similar themes were explored by Jorge Luis Borges, for instance in The Circular Ruins.

Modern popular culture often conceives of dreams, equally did Freud, equally expressions of the dreamer'southward deepest fears and desires.[78] In speculative fiction, the line between dreams and reality may be blurred even more in service to the story.[79] Dreams may be psychically invaded or manipulated (Dreamscape, 1984; the Nightmare on Elm Street films, 1984–2010; Inception, 2010) or fifty-fifty come literally truthful (as in The Lathe of Heaven, 1971).[78]

Lucidity

Lucid dreaming is the conscious perception of one'southward state while dreaming. In this state the dreamer may often have some degree of control over their own actions inside the dream or fifty-fifty the characters and the environs of the dream. Dream command has been reported to amend with practiced deliberate lucid dreaming, but the ability to command aspects of the dream is not necessary for a dream to authorize as "lucid"—a lucid dream is any dream during which the dreamer knows they are dreaming.[80] The occurrence of lucid dreaming has been scientifically verified.[81]

"Oneironaut" is a term sometimes used for those who lucidly dream.

In 1975, psychologist Keith Hearne successfully recorded a advice from a dreamer experiencing a lucid dream. On Apr 12, 1975, after agreeing to move his eyes left and correct upon becoming lucid, the subject and Hearne'south co-author on the resulting article, Alan Worsley, successfully carried out this task.[82] Years later, psychophysiologist Stephen LaBerge conducted similar piece of work including:

  • Using eye signals to map the subjective sense of fourth dimension in dreams.
  • Comparing the electric activity of the brain while singing awake and while dreaming.
  • Studies comparing in-dream sex, arousal, and orgasm.[83]

Communication between two dreamers has also been documented. The processes involved included EEG monitoring, ocular signaling, incorporation of reality in the form of red light stimuli and a coordinating website. The website tracked when both dreamers were dreaming and sent the stimulus to ane of the dreamers where it was incorporated into the dream. This dreamer, upon condign lucid, signaled with eye movements; this was detected by the website whereupon the stimulus was sent to the 2nd dreamer, invoking incorporation into that dreamer's dream.[84]

Recollection

The recollection of dreams is extremely unreliable, though it is a skill that tin can exist trained. Dreams tin normally exist recalled if a person is awakened while dreaming.[85] Women tend to have more than frequent dream recall than men.[85] Dreams that are difficult to recall may be characterized past relatively little touch on, and factors such every bit salience, arousal, and interference play a role in dream retrieve. Oft, a dream may be recalled upon viewing or hearing a random trigger or stimulus. The salience hypothesis proposes that dream content that is salient, that is, novel, intense, or unusual, is more easily remembered. There is considerable evidence that vivid, intense, or unusual dream content is more frequently recalled.[86] A dream journal tin be used to help dream recall, for personal involvement or psychotherapy purposes.

Adults written report remembering around two dreams per week, on boilerplate.[87] [88] Unless a dream is peculiarly vivid and if one wakes during or immediately after it, the content of the dream is typically not remembered.[89] Recording or reconstructing dreams may one day assist with dream recall. Using the permitted non-invasive technologies, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and electromyography (EMG), researchers have been able to identify basic dream imagery,[90] dream speech activity[91] and dream motor behavior (such as walking and paw movements).[92] [93]

In line with the salience hypothesis, there is considerable evidence that people who accept more than vivid, intense or unusual dreams prove better recollect. There is evidence that continuity of consciousness is related to remember. Specifically, people who have bright and unusual experiences during the day tend to accept more memorable dream content and hence better dream call up. People who score high on measures of personality traits associated with creativity, imagination, and fantasy, such as openness to feel, daydreaming, fantasy proneness, absorption, and hypnotic susceptibility, tend to show more than frequent dream recall.[86] There is also evidence for continuity betwixt the bizarre aspects of dreaming and waking experience. That is, people who report more bizarre experiences during the twenty-four hours, such every bit people loftier in schizotypy (psychosis proneness), accept more frequent dream recall and likewise written report more frequent nightmares.[86]

Miscellany

Illusion of reality

Some philosophers have proposed that what we think of as the "real world" could exist or is an illusion (an idea known equally the skeptical hypothesis about ontology). The first recorded mention of the idea was in the quaternary century BCE by Zhuangzi, and in Eastern philosophy, the problem has been named the "Zhuangzi Paradox."

He who dreams of drinking wine may cry when morning comes; he who dreams of weeping may in the morning become off to hunt. While he is dreaming he does not know information technology is a dream, and in his dream he may fifty-fifty try to interpret a dream. Merely after he wakes does he know it was a dream. And someday there will exist a great awakening when we know that this is all a bang-up dream. Yet the stupid believe they are awake, busily and brightly bold they understand things, calling this man ruler, that one herdsman—how dumbo! Confucius and you lot are both dreaming! And when I say you are dreaming, I am dreaming, too. Words similar these will exist labeled the Supreme Swindle. Yet, after ten thousand generations, a great sage may appear who will know their meaning, and it will still be as though he appeared with astonishing speed.[94]

The thought also is discussed in Hindu and Buddhist writings.[95] Information technology was formally introduced to Western philosophy past Descartes in the 17th century in his Meditations on Starting time Philosophy.

Absent-minded transgression

Dreams of absent transgression (DAMT) are dreams wherein the dreamer absent-minded-mindedly performs an activity that he or she has been trying to finish (i classic example is of a quitting smoker having dreams of lighting a cigarette). Subjects who have had DAMT have reported waking with intense feelings of guilt. 1 written report found a positive association betwixt having these dreams and successfully stopping the behavior.[96]

Daydreams

A daydream is a visionary fantasy, peculiarly one of happy, pleasant thoughts, hopes or ambitions, imagined equally coming to pass, and experienced while awake.[97] There are many different types of daydreams, and there is no consistent definition among psychologists.[97] The full general public also uses the term for a broad multifariousness of experiences. Research by Harvard psychologist Deirdre Barrett has found that people who feel vivid dreamlike mental images reserve the give-and-take for these, whereas many other people refer to milder imagery, realistic future planning, review of memories or just "spacing out"—i.due east. one'south mind going relatively blank—when they talk about "heedless."[98] [99]'

While daydreaming has long been derided as a lazy, not-productive pastime, it is at present usually best-selling that daydreaming can exist constructive in some contexts.[100] There are numerous examples of people in creative or creative careers, such equally composers, novelists and filmmakers, developing new ideas through daydreaming. Similarly, inquiry scientists, mathematicians and physicists have developed new ideas past daydreaming about their subject areas.

Hallucination

A hallucination, in the broadest sense of the word, is a perception in the absence of a stimulus. In a stricter sense, hallucinations are perceptions in a conscious and awake state, in the absence of external stimuli, and have qualities of real perception, in that they are vivid, substantial, and located in external objective space. The latter definition distinguishes hallucinations from the related phenomena of dreaming, which does not involve wakefulness.

Nightmare

Woman having a nightmare. Jean-Pierre Simon (1764–1810 or 1813).

A nightmare is an unpleasant dream that tin can cause a strong negative emotional response from the mind, typically fear or horror, but likewise despair, feet and cracking sadness. The dream may contain situations of danger, discomfort, psychological or physical terror. Sufferers usually awaken in a land of distress and may be unable to return to sleep for a prolonged catamenia of time.[101]

Nighttime terror

A night terror, as well known as a sleep terror or pavor nocturnus, is a parasomnia disorder that predominantly affects children, causing feelings of terror or dread. Night terrors should not exist confused with nightmares, which are bad dreams that cause the feeling of horror or fear.[102]

Déjà vu

One theory of déjà vu attributes the feeling of having previously seen or experienced something to having dreamed about a like situation or place, and forgetting most it until one seems to be mysteriously reminded of the situation or the identify while awake.[103]

See likewise

  • Cerebral neuroscience of dreams
  • Fantasize
  • Déjà vu
  • Dream argument
  • Dream fine art
  • Dream diary
  • Dream dictionary
  • Dream incubation
  • Dream interpretation
  • Dream of Macsen Wledig
  • Dream popular
  • Dream sequence
  • Dream oral communication
  • Dream world (plot device)
  • Dream Yoga
  • Dreamcatcher
  • Dreamwork
  • Fake awakening
  • Hallucination
  • Hatsuyume
  • Incubus
  • Lilith, a Sumerian dream demoness
  • List of dream diaries
  • Listing of dreams
  • Lucid dream
  • Mare (folklore)
  • Morpheus
  • Mabinogion
  • Neuroscience of slumber
  • Night terror
  • Nightmare
  • Oneirology
  • Oneiromancy
  • Precognition
  • Psychoanalysis
  • Rapid eye motility sleep
  • Sleep in not-human animals
  • Sleep paralysis
  • Spirit spouse
  • Succubus

References

  1. ^ "Dream". The American Heritage Dictionary of the English Linguistic communication, Fourth Edition. 2000. Retrieved May 7, 2009.
  2. ^ "Brain Basics: Understanding Slumber". National Found of Neurological Disorders and Stroke. 2006. Archived from the original on Oct 11, 2007. Retrieved December 16, 2007.
  3. ^ Lee Ann Obringer (2006). How Dream Works. Archived from the original on April 18, 2006. Retrieved May four, 2006.
  4. ^ Krippner, Stanley; Bogzaran, Fariba; Carvalho, Andre Percia de (2002). Extraordinary Dreams and How To Work with Them. Albany, NY: State University of New York Printing. p. 9. ISBN0-7914-5257-3. Dirt tablets have been found, dating to about 2500 B.C.Eastward., that contain interpretive material for Babylonian and Assyrian dreamers.
  5. ^ Seligman, Yard (1948). Magic, Supernaturalism and Organized religion. New York: Random Business firm.
  6. ^ a b c d Black, Jeremy; Light-green, Anthony (1992). Gods, Demons and Symbols of Ancient Mesopotamia: An Illustrated Dictionary. Austin: University of Texas Press. pp. 71–72, 89–90. ISBN0714117056.
  7. ^ a b Freud, Sigmund (1965). James Strachey (ed.). The Interpretation of Dreams. Translated by James Strachey. New York: Avon.
  8. ^ Schredl, Michael; Bohusch, Claudia; Kahl, Johanna; Mader, Andrea; Somesan, Alexandra (2000). "The Utilize of Dreams in Psychotherapy". The Journal of Psychotherapy Practice and Research. 9 (2): 81–87.
  9. ^ Kavanau, J.L. (2000). "Slumber, memory maintenance, and mental disorders". Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences. 12 (ii): 199–208. doi:10.1176/jnp.12.two.199. PMID 11001598.
  10. ^ a b Dodds, East. R. (1951). The Greeks and the Irrational. Berkeley: Academy of California Printing. p. 105. The Greeks never spoke equally we do of having a dream, simply always of seeing a dream....
  11. ^ a b Packer, Sharon (2002). Dreams in Myth, Medicine, and Movies. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers. p. 85. ISBN0-275-97243-seven. …[Grand]any more ancient cultures think that dreams are imposed past a force that resides outside the private.
  12. ^ Macrobius (1952) [430]. Commentary on the Dream of Scipio. Translated past W. H. Stahl. New York: Columbia Academy Press. p. xc. We call a dream oracular in which a parent, or a pious or revered human being, or a priest, or even a god clearly reveals what volition or will not transpire, and what activeness to take or to avoid.
  13. ^ Dodds (1951), referring to the blazon of dream described past Macrobius: "This last type is not, I call up, at all common in our own dream-feel. Merely there is considerable show that dreams of this sort were familiar in antiquity." (p. 107).
  14. ^ a b Krippner, Stanley; Bogzaran, Fariba; Carvalho, André Percia de (2002). Boggling Dreams and How To Piece of work with Them. Albany: State University of New York Printing. p. 10. ISBN0-7914-5257-3. The Egyptian papyrus of Deral-Madineh was written about 1300 B.C.E. and gives instructions on how to obtain a dream bulletin from a god.
  15. ^ Lesku, J. A.; Meyer, L. C. R.; Fuller, A.; Maloney, S. K.; Dell'Omo, G.; Vyssotski, A. L.; Rattenborg, Due north. C. (2011). "Ostriches sleep like platypuses". PLOS One. 6 (8): 1–7. Bibcode:2011PLoSO...623203L. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0023203. PMC3160860. PMID 21887239.
  16. ^ a b Solms, Marker (2000). "Dreaming and REM sleep are controlled by different encephalon mechanisms". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 23 (6): 843–850. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00003988. PMID 11515144. S2CID 7264870. Dreaming and REM sleep are incompletely correlated. Between five and 30% of REM awakenings do non elicit dream reports; and at least 5–ten% of NREM awakenings exercise elicit dream reports that are indistinguishable from REM....
  17. ^ Bulkeley, Kelly (2008). Dreaming in the world'southward religions: A comparative history. p. fourteen. ISBN978-0-8147-9956-7. Practise animals dream? We currently have no ways of proving information technology one way or the other, just as we accept no way to determine whether man fetuses and newborns are genuinely dreaming before they develop the ability to speak and relate their experiences.
  18. ^ Damasio, Antonio (2010). Self Comes to Mind. New York: Pantheon Books. p. 289. ISBN978-0-307-37875-0. …I sympathize with Julian Jaynes's claim that something of swell import may have happened to the man mind during the relatively brief interval of fourth dimension betwixt the events narrated in the Iliad and those that make up the Odyssey.
  19. ^ Nielsen, Tore A. (1991), "Reality Dreams and Their Effects on Spiritual Belief: A Revision of Animism Theory", in Gackenbach, Jayne; Sheikh, Anees A. (eds.), Dream Images: A Call to Mental Arms, Amityville, NY: Baywood, pp. 233–264, ISBN0-89503-056-X
  20. ^ Atwan, Robert (1981). "The Interpretation of Dreams, The Origin of Consciousness, and the Nascency of Tragedy". Enquiry Advice in Psychology, Psychiatry and Beliefs. vi (2): 163–182.
  21. ^ a b c Hall, C., & Van de Castle, R. (1966). The Content Assay of Dreams. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Content Analysis Explained Archived 2007-04-12 at the Wayback Machine
  22. ^ Schneider, Adam; Domhoff, M. William. "The Classification and Coding of Characters". University of California at Santa Cruz. Retrieved July 17, 2021.
  23. ^ Schredl, Michael; Ciric, Petra; Götz, Simon; Wittmann, Lutz (November 2004). "Typical Dreams: Stability and Gender Differences". The Journal of Psychology. 138 (vi): 485–494. doi:10.3200/JRLP.138.6.485-494. PMID 15612605. S2CID 13554573.
  24. ^ Zadra, A., "1093: Sex dreams: what practice men and women dream about?" Archived 2007-09-27 at the Wayback Automobile, Sleep Volume 30, Abstract Supplement, 2007 A376.
  25. ^ "Badan Pusat Statistik "Indonesia Young Adult Reproductive Wellness Survey 2002–2004" p. 27" (PDF). Archived (PDF) from the original on Dec ix, 2012. Retrieved April 4, 2013.
  26. ^ "How do bullheaded people dream? – The Body Odd". March 2012. Archived from the original on January 24, 2013. Retrieved May x, 2013.
  27. ^ Hobson, J. Allan; Pace-Schott, Edward F.; Stickgold, Robert (2000). "Dream scientific discipline 2000: A response to commentaries on Dreaming and the Brain". Behavioral and Brain Sciences. 23 (6): 1019. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00954025. S2CID 144729368.
  28. ^ Chiong, Winston; Leonard, Matthew K.; Chang, Edward F. (2018). "Neurosurgical Patients as Human Research Subjects: Ethical Considerations in Intracranial Electrophysiology Enquiry". Neurosurgery. 83 (i): 29–37. doi:10.1093/neuros/nyx361. PMC5777911. PMID 28973530.
  29. ^ a b Hobson, J. A., Pace-Schott, E. F., & Stickgold, R. (2000). "Dreaming and the brain: Toward a cognitive neuroscience of conscious states". Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(vi), 793–842.
  30. ^ Ringach, Dario L. (30 July 2009). "The limits of fMRI". Speaking of Inquiry. Retrieved July 18, 2021.
  31. ^ "Policies on the Utilize of Animals and Humans in Inquiry". Club for Neuroscience. Retrieved July 18, 2021.
  32. ^ Uttal, William R. (2013). Reliability in Cognitive Neuroscience. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press. p. four. Similarly, modern neuroscience enquiry is increasingly showing that activation areas on the brain associated with a cognitive process are far more than widely distributed than had been thought only a decade or so agone. Indeed, information technology at present seems likely that well-nigh of the brain is agile in virtually any cognitive process.
  33. ^ Solms, Mark (2000). "Dreaming and REM slumber are controlled past unlike brain mechanisms". Behavioral and Encephalon Sciences. 23 (six): 843–850, word 904–1121. doi:ten.1017/S0140525X00003988. PMID 11515144. S2CID 7264870.
  34. ^ Braun, A. R.; Balkin, T. J; Wesensten, N. J.; Carson, R. E.; Varga, One thousand.; Baldwin, P.; Selbie, Southward.; Belenky, G.; Herscovitch, P. (1997). "Regional cognitive claret menstruation through the sleep-wake cycle". Brain. Oxford University Printing. 120: 1173–1197. doi:10.1093/brain/120.7.1173. PMID 9236630.
  35. ^ Kalat, James W. (2015). Biological Psychology (12 ed.). Boston: Cengage. p. 288. ISBN978-1305105409.
  36. ^ Viskontas, Indre (2017). Brain Myths Exploded: Lessons from Neuroscience. Chantilly, VA: The Didactics Visitor. p. 393.
  37. ^ Lévy-Bruhl, Lucien (1923). "Chapter III Dreams". Primitive Mentality. Translated by Lilian A. Clare. New York: Macmillan. pp. 97–121.
  38. ^ Robert, Westward. Der Traum als Naturnothwendigkeit erklärt. Zweite Auflage, Hamburg: Seippel, 1886.
  39. ^ Freud, Sigmund (1965). James Strachey (ed.). The Interpretation of Dreams. Translated past James Strachey. New York: Avon. p. 188. The view adopted past Robert [1886, 9 f.] that the purpose of dreams is to unburden our memory of the useless impressions of daytime [cf. pp. 105 f.] is plainly no longer tenable....
  40. ^ Rycroft, Charles. A Critical Dictionary of Psychoanalysis. London: Penguin Books, 1995, p. 41.
  41. ^ Freud, Sigmund (1965). James Strachey (ed.). The Interpretation of Dreams. Translated by James Strachey. New York: Avon. p. 253.
  42. ^ Aserinsky, Eugene; Kleitman, Nathaniel (1953). "Regularly Occurring Periods of Eye Motility, and Concomitant Phenomena, during Sleep". Science. 118 (3062): 273–274. Bibcode:1953Sci...118..273A. doi:ten.1126/science.118.3062.273. PMID 13089671.
  43. ^ Smith, Robert C. (1991), "The Pregnant of Dreams: A Current Alert Theory", in Gackenbach, Jayne; Sheikh, Anees A. (eds.), Dream Images: A Phone call to Mental Artillery, Amityville, NY: Baywood, pp. 127–146, ISBN0-89503-056-X
  44. ^ Hobson, J. Allan; McCarley, Robert Due west. (December 1977). "The Brain as a Dream State Generator: An Activation-Synthesis Hypothesis of the Dream Process". The American Journal of Psychiatry. 134 (12): 1335–1348. doi:10.1176/ajp.134.12.1335. PMID 21570. The dream process is thus seen as having its origin in sensorimotor systems, with piffling or no primary ideational, volitional, or emotional content. This concept is markedly different from that of the "dream thoughts" or wishes seen by Freud as the primary stimulus for the dream.
  45. ^ Benjamin, Victoria. "Report Links Dreaming to Increased Memory Performance". The Harvard Cherry-red. The Harvard Crimson. Retrieved 27 January 2022.
  46. ^ Evans, C.; Newman, Eastward. (1964). "Dreaming: An illustration from computers". New Scientist. 419: 577–579.
  47. ^ Crick, F.; Mitchison, G. (1983). "The function of dream sleep". Nature. 304 (5922): 111–114. Bibcode:1983Natur.304..111C. doi:x.1038/304111a0. PMID 6866101. S2CID 41500914.
  48. ^ Hartmann, Ernest (1995). "Making Connections in a Rubber Identify: Is Dreaming Psychotherapy?". Dreaming. v (4): 213–228. doi:10.1037/h0094437.
  49. ^ Revonsuo, A. (2000). "The reinterpretation of dreams: an evolutionary hypothesis of the part of dreaming". Behavioral and Encephalon Sciences. 23 (6): 877–901. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00004015. PMID 11515147. S2CID 145340071.
  50. ^ Eagleman, David G.; Vaughn, Don A. (May 2021). "The Defensive Activation Theory: REM Sleep equally a Mechanism to Prevent Takeover of the Visual Cortex". Frontiers in Neuroscience. fifteen: 632853. doi:10.3389/fnins.2021.632853. PMC8176926. PMID 34093109.
  51. ^ Lévy-Bruhl, Lucien (1923). "Chapter III Dreams". Primitive Mentality. Translated by Lilian A. Clare. New York: Macmillan. p. 98. ...[I]n dreams,...human passes from the one world to the other without existence aware of information technology. Such is in fact the ordinary idea of the dream to primitive peoples. The "soul" leaves its tenement for the time existence. Information technology frequently goes very far away; it communes with spirits or with ghosts. At the moment of enkindling information technology returns to take its place in the trunk once more.
  52. ^ Dunne, J. Westward. (1950) [1927]. An Experiment with Fourth dimension. London: Faber. p. 23.
  53. ^ Krishnananda, Swami (xvi November 1996). "The Mandukya Upanishad, Department 4". Archived from the original on 9 April 2015. Retrieved 26 March 2015.
  54. ^ Bar, Shaul (2001). A letter that has not been read: Dreams in the Hebrew Bible. Hebrew Union College Press. Retrieved April four, 2013.
  55. ^ Edgar, Iain (2011). The Dream in Islam: From Qur'anic Tradition to Jihadist Inspiration. Oxford: Berghahn Books. p. 178. ISBN978-0-85745-235-one. Archived from the original on 2011-09-29. Retrieved 2012-03-09 .
  56. ^ a b c d Edgar, Iain R.; Henig, David (September 2010). "Istikhara: The Guidance and practise of Islamic dream incubation through ethnographic comparison" (PDF). History and Anthropology. 21 (3): 251–262. CiteSeerXx.1.1.1012.7334. doi:ten.1080/02757206.2010.496781. S2CID 144463607. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2017-xi-08. Retrieved 2017-10-26 .
  57. ^ a b Young, Southward. (2003). "Dreams". The encyclopedia of South Asian Folklore. Vol. xiii. p. 7. Archived from the original on 2018-02-25. Retrieved 2018-02-24 – via Indian Folklife.
  58. ^ a b Bulkeley, Kelly (2008). Dreaming in the earth's religions: A comparative history . pp. 71–73. ISBN978-0-8147-9956-7.
  59. ^ Oppenheim, L.A. (1966). Mantic Dreams in the Aboriginal Well-nigh Eastward in One thousand. E. Von Grunebaum & R. Caillois (Eds.), The Dream and Human Societies (pp. 341–350). London, England: Cambridge University Press.
  60. ^ Nils P. Heessel : Divinatorische Texte I : ... oneiromantische Omina. Harrassowitz Verlag, 2007.
  61. ^ O'Neil, C.W. (1976). Dreams, culture and the individual. San Francisco: Chandler & Sharp.
  62. ^ Cicero, De Republica, vi.ten
  63. ^ Herodotus (1998). The Histories . Oxford University Printing. p. 414.
  64. ^ Uluru – Kata Tjuta National Park: Tjukurpa – Anangu culture Archived July 11, 2009, at the Wayback Machine environment.gov.au, June 23, 2006
  65. ^ Tedlock, B (1981). "Quiche Maya dream Estimation". Ethos. 9 (4): 313–350. doi:10.1525/eth.1981.ix.four.02a00050.
  66. ^ Webb, Craig (1995). "Dreams: Practical Meaning & Applications". The DREAMS Foundation. Archived from the original on 2016-03-05. Retrieved 2008-03-30 .
  67. ^ "Native American Dream Beliefs". Dream Encyclopedia. Archived from the original on Apr 15, 2012. Retrieved April 10, 2012.
  68. ^ Morewedge, Carey K.; Norton, Michael I. (2009). "When dreaming is believing: The (motivated) interpretation of dreams" (PDF). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 96 (2): 249–264. doi:x.1037/a0013264. PMID 19159131. S2CID 5706448. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2020-11-xiv.
  69. ^ a b Hines, Terence (2003). Pseudoscience and the Paranormal. Prometheus Books. pp. 78–81. ISBN978-ane-57392-979-0.
  70. ^ Gilovich, Thomas (1991). How We Know What Isn't And then: the fallibility of human reason in everyday life. Simon & Schuster. pp. 177–180. ISBN978-0-02-911706-4.
  71. ^ "Llewellyn Worldwide – Encyclopedia: Term: Veridical Dream". www.llewellyn.com. Archived from the original on 2016-10-18. Retrieved 2016-x-16 .
  72. ^ Alcock, James Due east. (1981). Parapsychology: Scientific discipline or Magic?: a psychological perspective. Oxford: Pergamon Press. ISBN978-0-08-025773-0. via Hines, Terence (2003). Pseudoscience and the Paranormal. Prometheus Books. pp. 78–81. ISBN978-one-57392-979-0.
  73. ^ Madey, Scott; Thomas Gilovich (1993). "Effects of Temporal Focus on the Recall of Expectancy-Consistent and Expectancy-Inconsistent Information". Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. 65 (3): 458–468. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.65.3.458. PMID 8410650. via Kida, Thomas (2006). Don't Believe Everything You Retrieve: The 6 Basic Mistakes Nosotros Make in Thinking. Prometheus Books. ISBN978-1-59102-408-viii.
  74. ^ "The book of the duchess". Washington State University. Archived from the original on November 14, 2012. Retrieved May 24, 2012.
  75. ^ "William Langland's The Vision Apropos Piers Plowman". The History Guide. Archived from the original on June 6, 2012. Retrieved May 24, 2012.
  76. ^ Phillips Lovecraft, Howard (1995). The Dream Bicycle of H. P. Lovecraft: Dreams of Terror and Death. Ballantine Books. ISBN978-0-345-38421-8. Archived from the original on 2013-09-03.
  77. ^ "The Neverending Story – Book – Pictures – Video – Icons". Archived from the original on June 1, 2012. Retrieved May 24, 2012.
  78. ^ a b Van Riper, A. Bowdoin (2002). Scientific discipline in popular culture: a reference guide. Westport: Greenwood Printing. pp. 56–57. ISBN978-0-313-31822-one.
  79. ^ Van Riper, op. cit., p. 57.
  80. ^ Lucid dreaming FAQ Archived 2007-03-xiii at the Wayback Automobile past The Lucidity Constitute at Psych Web.
  81. ^ Watanabe, T. (2003). "Lucid Dreaming: Its Experimental Proof and Psychological Weather". J Int Soc Life Inf Sci. 21 (one). ISSN 1341-9226.
  82. ^ "Archived re-create" (PDF). Archived from the original (PDF) on 2012-04-26. Retrieved 2013-ten-21 . {{cite spider web}}: CS1 maint: archived copy as title (link), Lucid Dream Communication
  83. ^ LaBerge, S. (2014). Lucid dreaming: Paradoxes of dreaming consciousness. In E. Cardeña, S. Lynn, Due south. Krippner (Eds.), Varieties of anomalous feel: Examining the scientific testify (2nd ed.) (pp. 145–173). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. doi:10.1037/14258-006
  84. ^ Olson, Parmy. "Saying 'Hello' Through A Dream: How The Internet Could Make Sleeping More Social". Forbes. Archived from the original on 2016-05-31. Retrieved 2016-04-24 .
  85. ^ a b "The Science Behind Dreams and Nightmares". Npr.org. Archived from the original on August 22, 2013. Retrieved Apr four, 2013.
  86. ^ a b c Watson, David (2003). "To dream, perchance to remember: Private differences in dream recall". Personality and Private Differences. 34 (7): 1271–1286. doi:10.1016/S0191-8869(02)00114-9.
  87. ^ "Why Do Some People Always Think Their Dreams, While Others Nigh Never Do?". Discover Magazine. 2019. Retrieved 10 February 2021.
  88. ^ Herlin, Bastien; Leu-Semenescu, Smaranda; Chaumereuil, Charlotte; Arnulf, Isabelle (December 2015). "Evidence that non-dreamers do dream: a REM sleep behaviour disorder model". Journal of Slumber Research. 24 (six): 602–609. doi:10.1111/jsr.12323. PMID 26307463. Adults report, on boilerplate, one–2.8 dream recalls per week in a dream questionnaire and 2.38 dream recalls per week when a home dream diary is completed
  89. ^ Hobson, J.A.; McCarly, R.Due west. (1977). "The brain as a dream-state generator: An activation-synthesis hypothesis of the dream process" (PDF). American Journal of Psychiatry. 134 (12): 1335–1348. doi:10.1176/ajp.134.12.1335. PMID 21570. S2CID 10396934. Archived from the original (PDF) on 2017-07-09.
  90. ^ Morelle, Rebecca (2013-04-04). "Scientists 'read dreams' using brain scans". BBC News. Archived from the original on 2016-04-27. Retrieved 2016-04-24 .
  91. ^ Mcguigan, F. (2012). The Psychophysiology of Thinking: Studies of Covert Processes. Elsevier. ISBN978-0-323-14700-ii.
  92. ^ Cvetkovic, Dean; Cosic, Irena (2011). States of Consciousness: Experimental Insights into Meditation, Waking, Slumber and Dreams. Springer Science & Business Media. ISBN978-3-642-18047-vii.
  93. ^ Oldis, Daniel (2016-02-04). "Tin Nosotros Turn Our Dreams Into Watchable Movies?". The Huffington Post. Archived from the original on 2016-08-04. Retrieved 2016-08-20 .
  94. ^ 莊子, 齊物論, 12. Zhuàngzi, "Word on making all things equal," 12. from Zhuàngzi, Burton Watson trans., Chuang Tzu (New York: Columbia University Press, 1996), 43. ISBN 978-0-231-10595-8 [1]
  95. ^ Kher, Chitrarekha V. (1992). Buddhism Every bit Presented by the Brahmanical Systems. Sri Satguru Publications. ISBN978-81-7030-293-iii.
  96. ^ Hajek P, Belcher M (1991). "Dream of absent-minded transgression: an empirical report of a cerebral withdrawal symptom". J Abnorm Psychol. 100 (4): 487–491. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.100.4.487. PMID 1757662.
  97. ^ a b Klinger, Eric (October 1987). Psychology Today.
  98. ^ Barrett, D.L. (1979). "The Hypnotic Dream: Its Content in Comparison to Nocturnal Dreams and Waking Fantasy". Journal of Aberrant Psychology. 88 (5): 584–591. doi:10.1037/0021-843x.88.5.584.
  99. ^ Barrett, D.L. "Fantasizers and Dissociaters: 2 types of Loftier Hypnotizables, Two Imagery Styles". in R. Kusendorf, N. Spanos, & B. Wallace (Eds.) Hypnosis and Imagination. New York: Baywood, 1996. and, Barrett, D.L. "Dissociaters, Fantasizers, and their Relation to Hypnotizability" in Barrett, D.L. (Ed.) Hypnosis and Hypnotherapy, (ii vol.): Vol. 1: History, theory and general research, Vol. 2: Psychotherapy enquiry and applications, New York: Praeger/Greenwood, 2010.
  100. ^ Tierney, John (June 28, 2010). "Discovering the Virtues of a Wandering Listen". The New York Times. Archived from the original on April 21, 2017.
  101. ^ American Psychiatric Association (2000), Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th ed, TR, p. 631
  102. ^ Hockenbury, Don H. Hockenbury, Sandra Due east. (2010). Discovering psychology (5th ed.). New York: Worth Publishers. p. 157. ISBN978-1-4292-1650-0.
  103. ^ Lohff, David C. (2004). The Dream Directory: The Comprehensive Guide to Analysis and Estimation. Running Printing. ISBN978-0-7624-1962-3.

Further reading

  • Dreaming (journal)
  • Jung, Carl (1934). The Practice of Psychotherapy. "The Practical Use of Dream-analysis" . New York: Routledge & Kegan Paul. pp. 139–. ISBN978-0-7100-1645-4.
  • Jung, Carl (2002). Dreams (Routledge Classics). New York: Routledge. ISBN978-0-415-26740-3.
  • Harris, William V. (2009) Dreams and Еxperience in Classical Antiquity. Cambridge, MA & London: Harvard University Press.

External links

  • Dreams on In Our Time at the BBC
  • LSDBase – an online sleep research database documenting the physiological effects of dreams through biofeedback.
  • Archive for Research in Archetypal Symbolism website
  • The International Association for the Study of Dreams
  • Dream at Curlie
  • Dixit, Jay (November 2007). "Dreams: Nighttime School". Psychology Today . Retrieved December 1, 2018.
  • alt.dreams A long-running USENET forum wherein readers postal service and analyze dreams.

couchjoiny1968.blogspot.com

Source: https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Dream

0 Response to "I Dreamt of Meangaling With People I Have Been Ask Not to See Again What Does That Mean"

Post a Comment

Iklan Atas Artikel

Iklan Tengah Artikel 1

Iklan Tengah Artikel 2

Iklan Bawah Artikel